Insectopedia - BestLightNovel.com
You’re reading novel Insectopedia Part 7 online at BestLightNovel.com. Please use the follow button to get notification about the latest chapter next time when you visit BestLightNovel.com. Use F11 button to read novel in full-screen(PC only). Drop by anytime you want to read free – fast – latest novel. It’s great if you could leave a comment, share your opinion about the new chapters, new novel with others on the internet. We’ll do our best to bring you the finest, latest novel everyday. Enjoy
So even though Jeff likes to sign off as Jeff "The Bug" Vilencia, his aspiration is really quite modest: he just wants what a bug's got-worthlessness, repulsiveness, vulnerability, squis.h.i.+ness. It's not a huge transformation. He already has most of this. And he's managed to find a positive value in it, discovered that, for him, humiliation is the fulfillment of desire. He can pay women to walk on him. But he needs more. He needs his bug-like nature to be made visible, and he needs to be forced to suffer the consequences-again and again and again.
And that's why I'm guessing he's not really a guy-bug/bug-guy, because, by necessity, none of this suffering and humiliation produces empathy or sympathy for the insect. How could it? Because suffering is pleasure and because the insect is just a container for all that dark pleasure, nothing more. The insect is the dark place that sucks up society's disgust. It is the anonymous dark place that enables relentless repet.i.tion. Squish, squish, squish. Like a baby throwing its bottle to the ground again and again, every time it's picked up, again and again, trying to figure out something that's all at once obscure and vacant. Again and again. Nothing more.
Are you feeling it? That's what counts here. Don't worry about why they're doing this, even though crush freaks themselves-cursed with the explanatory burden of a "minority s.e.xuality"-have little choice but to fret about that all the time. b.a.s.t.a.r.d child of foot fetis.h.i.+sm, reject infant of giantessism, downcast cousin of trampling, alienated half sibling of zoophilia, evil twin of the messy thing.10 Everyone traces it to childhood, to an unantic.i.p.ated glimpse of an unfortunate episode: mother, insect, foot. In that blink of a wide-open eye, something gets made forever, something gets lost forever. Everyone traces it to childhood, to an unantic.i.p.ated glimpse of an unfortunate episode: mother, insect, foot. In that blink of a wide-open eye, something gets made forever, something gets lost forever.
To Freud, fetis.h.i.+sm is a disavowal, "an oscillation between two logically incompatible beliefs."11 The impossibility of resolution produces the constant return-to the foot, to the insect, to the explosive death, to the moment long ago before that bad thing happened. To the absent female phallus. Or maybe not. It doesn't seem wholly serious when you write it down like that. The impossibility of resolution produces the constant return-to the foot, to the insect, to the explosive death, to the moment long ago before that bad thing happened. To the absent female phallus. Or maybe not. It doesn't seem wholly serious when you write it down like that.
Still, it's not just crush freaks who need to know. As we'll soon see, everyone wants an origin story, everyone from Fox TV to the D.A.'s office to the Humane Society to the Judiciary Committee of the U.S. House of Representatives. Why this need to unravel causation? To make sure it doesn't happen again? To develop a cure? To nullify, justify, pathologize, normalize, criminalize? There's agreement on all sides that this stomping is a symptom of something gone wrong. The only symptoms no one feels compelled to explain are the ones revealed in these inescapable demands for explanation.
Like most of us, Jeff is thoroughly inconsistent. Unlike most of us, he is highly quotable. "At this point in my life," he wrote in the Journal Journal, "I am more interested in the thing itself rather than its origin."12 The bug gets squished. The man gets off. That's what counts. Maybe you're not feeling it. Jeff is feeling it. The bug gets squished. The man gets off. That's what counts. Maybe you're not feeling it. Jeff is feeling it.
Georges Bataille begins his inspiringly unapologetic picture book, The Tears of Eros The Tears of Eros, in the voice of the utopian manifesto. "We are finally," he announces, "beginning to see the absurdity of any connection between eroticism and morality." Morality, he tells us later, "makes the value of an act depend on its consequences."13 With the arrival of People v. Thomason People v. Thomason in the summer of 1999, Jeff Vilencia, America's only telegenic crush freak, found himself back in the media spotlight. But this time, everything was different. It wasn't only hapless Gary Thomason who was making headlines. Crush freaks were also keeping cops busy in Islip Terrace, a suburb of Long Island. Acting on a tip from Thomas Capriola's ex-girlfriend, the police raided the twenty-seven-year-old's bedroom and found a half dozen semiautomatic firearms, a poster of a n.a.z.i storm trooper, a fish tank full of mice, a pair of high heels coated in dried blood, and-the items that disturbed them most of all-seventy-one crush videos, which, Suffolk County prosecutors claimed, Capriola was selling through his Crush G.o.ddess website and ads in p.o.r.n magazines. in the summer of 1999, Jeff Vilencia, America's only telegenic crush freak, found himself back in the media spotlight. But this time, everything was different. It wasn't only hapless Gary Thomason who was making headlines. Crush freaks were also keeping cops busy in Islip Terrace, a suburb of Long Island. Acting on a tip from Thomas Capriola's ex-girlfriend, the police raided the twenty-seven-year-old's bedroom and found a half dozen semiautomatic firearms, a poster of a n.a.z.i storm trooper, a fish tank full of mice, a pair of high heels coated in dried blood, and-the items that disturbed them most of all-seventy-one crush videos, which, Suffolk County prosecutors claimed, Capriola was selling through his Crush G.o.ddess website and ads in p.o.r.n magazines.14 Suddenly, America was in a pincer grip. Seeping across the map like the red tide in a cold war animation, crush freaks were advancing on the heartland from both coasts. Someone had to stand and fight. Michael Bradbury, the Ventura County district attorney, held a press conference along with representatives of the Doris Day Animal League. On a sunny day in Simi Valley, in front of large-format images of insects, kittens, guinea pigs, and mice being squashed under women's feet, they launched the campaign to fast-track House Resolution 1887, a federal bill designed to criminalize the production and distribution of crush videos.15 The sponsor of the bill was Representative Elton Gallegly, a seven-term California Republican known for his energetic support of the citrus and wine industries' campaign to eradicate the gla.s.sy-winged sharpshooter leafhopper (as well as a record on immigration so hawkish that it led to his induction into the U.S. Border Patrol Hall of Fame). The sponsor of the bill was Representative Elton Gallegly, a seven-term California Republican known for his energetic support of the citrus and wine industries' campaign to eradicate the gla.s.sy-winged sharpshooter leafhopper (as well as a record on immigration so hawkish that it led to his induction into the U.S. Border Patrol Hall of Fame).16 Gallegly described the fetish as "one of the sickest and most demented forms of animal cruelty that I've ever been exposed to." Gallegly described the fetish as "one of the sickest and most demented forms of animal cruelty that I've ever been exposed to."
The campaign presented crush as a "gateway fetish." Just as cannabis leads inexorably to crack, its spokespeople argued, fetis.h.i.+sts might begin innocuously enough with grapes and worms, but-step by step-they will be drawn up the ladder of Creation, until, in Bradbury's lurid scenario, it won't be long before someone "pay[s] $1 million to have a kid crushed."17 To underline the point, one of his deputies testified to having seen a video in which a baby doll was trampled underfoot. Seventy-eight-year-old former child star Mickey Rooney piped up. "Put a stop, won't you, to crush videos," he begged. "What are we gonna hand our children? This is what we're going to hand down, these videos, crush videos? G.o.d forbid." To underline the point, one of his deputies testified to having seen a video in which a baby doll was trampled underfoot. Seventy-eight-year-old former child star Mickey Rooney piped up. "Put a stop, won't you, to crush videos," he begged. "What are we gonna hand our children? This is what we're going to hand down, these videos, crush videos? G.o.d forbid."18 As the bill headed to Congress, Jeff became the go-to guy for the entire media. For a few intense weeks, he was inundated with requests from radio stations, magazines, and newspapers. Perhaps seduced by that peculiarly American brew of idealism, exhibitionism, and celebrity seeking, he ignored the advice of a lawyer friend. Perhaps naively, he made himself available to all requests. ("I thought, Well, that's not fair because, first of all, we didn't do anything wrong....") Still, for a while at least, he was able to give himself some cover. In interviews, he declared that he had ceased production of videos until all legal questions were resolved, and he drew a sharp line between the "vermin" featured in his own movies-specifically insects-and the mammals with which Bradbury, Gallegly, and Rooney were especially preoccupied. He didn't believe crush videos of domestic mammals actually existed, he said, but if they did, he certainly had no interest in them. I at first a.s.sumed this demarcation was a legalistic maneuver Jeff was using to protect himself in the midst of a dangerous moral panic. But then I realized it was a distinction fundamental to his fetish. Of course, he maintained, he had no interest in stomping on domestic pets. Even rodents, he told the a.s.sociated Press, are "too furry, too animal-looking."19 Jeff's argument was the same now as it had been on the talk show circuit in 1993. He took aim at those like Tom Connors, the Ventura County deputy D.A., who claimed it was the method, not the fact, of killing animals that was at issue. In Jeff's view, it was the very fact of killing animals that was wrong. The method was irrelevant. His critique was systemic. ("Look," he told me, "America's seventy-five percent grossly obese-you don't think they got there eating f.u.c.kin' vegetables?") Killing animals was endemic to capitalist society. What was at stake in the fight over crush videos, he argued, was the hypocrisy of a society that turned a blind eye to the daily ma.s.s slaughter of all kinds of animals but threw up its arms in horror when a tiny number of people killed for s.e.xual pleasure. Jeff, it turned out, was a vegan and an animal rights activist.
"What about the fur industry, what about fishermen, what about the cattle industry?" he asked the BBC. "You can kill anything you want, basically, in any manner you want if it's for food or for sport or for fas.h.i.+on, but you cross the line when you do it for s.e.xual gratification."20 And anyway, he added, if we're honest, don't we all know that the excitement of the bullfighter and the thrill of the hunter is a s.e.xual thrill, a s.e.xual excitement. They're getting off on killing. The problem here is that crush freaks don't pretend otherwise. "I thought," Jeff told me, "I'm going to tell the world it might be reprehensible and gross, but it's not anything worse than what everybody does on a day-to-day basis." Or as he put it in a live encounter with Elton Gallegly on Court TV: "Our fine Congressman says there's a humane way to kill vermin. That's a colloquialism. Killing is killing. You kill them fast, you kill them slow. I wonder if the Congressman has ever seen a sticky trap or a snap trap. There's nothing humane about them." And anyway, he added, if we're honest, don't we all know that the excitement of the bullfighter and the thrill of the hunter is a s.e.xual thrill, a s.e.xual excitement. They're getting off on killing. The problem here is that crush freaks don't pretend otherwise. "I thought," Jeff told me, "I'm going to tell the world it might be reprehensible and gross, but it's not anything worse than what everybody does on a day-to-day basis." Or as he put it in a live encounter with Elton Gallegly on Court TV: "Our fine Congressman says there's a humane way to kill vermin. That's a colloquialism. Killing is killing. You kill them fast, you kill them slow. I wonder if the Congressman has ever seen a sticky trap or a snap trap. There's nothing humane about them."21 Gallegly's bill sailed through Congress by a vote of 37242 in the House and unanimous acclaim in the Senate. Nonetheless, there was significant unease about the law's First Amendment implications. This was a bill written to criminalize content-"the depiction of animal cruelty"-and before it reached the floor of the House, it was substantially revised in the Judiciary Committee's Subcommittee on Crime to allow exceptions "if the material in question has serious religious, political, scientific, educational, journalistic, historic, or artistic value."22 Despite that, a number of representatives, most notably Robert Scott (D-Va.), argued strongly that the bill was still too broad ("Films of animals being crushed are communications about the acts depicted, not doing the acts") and failed to demonstrate compelling government interest (a test established by the Supreme Court in 1988 for First Amendment cases).23 On this point, it was clear from the Supreme Court's upholding of the rights of the Santeria church of Luk.u.mi Babalu Aye in 1993 against a Hialeah, Florida, city ordinance banning animal sacrifice that, despite the arguments of animal rights activists, the welfare of animals was not recognized in law as sufficient grounds to restrict First Amendment speech. On this point, it was clear from the Supreme Court's upholding of the rights of the Santeria church of Luk.u.mi Babalu Aye in 1993 against a Hialeah, Florida, city ordinance banning animal sacrifice that, despite the arguments of animal rights activists, the welfare of animals was not recognized in law as sufficient grounds to restrict First Amendment speech.24 So what might const.i.tute the compelling state interest in crush videos? Representative after representative rose in support of Gallegly's bill to secure the link between violence toward animals and violence toward people. They invoked spousal abuse, elder abuse, child abuse, and even school shootings. Congressman Spencer Bachus (R-Ala.) summarized the logic of this animal protection legislation most succinctly of all: "This is about children," he informed the Speaker of the House, "not about beetles."25 Still, the real headline grabbers were the celebrity serial killers. What did Ted Bundy, Ted "the Unabomber" Kaczynski, and David "Son of Sam" Berkowitz have in common? Gallegly had the answer: "They all tortured or killed animals before they started killing people."26 It made good copy, but I doubt even the politicians believed this link between crush videos and ma.s.s murder. After all, many of them had already listened to Susan "Minnie" Creede, the D.A.'s undercover investigator, as she testified before the Subcommittee on Crime about the psychology of the crush fetis.h.i.+st. After almost a year in the Crushcentral chat room, Creede was an expert witness.
"They spoke about their fetishes and how they developed," she told the committee.
For many of them the fetish developed as a result of something they saw at a very early age, and it usually occurred before the age of five. Most of these men saw a woman step on something. She was usually someone who was significantly in their lives. They were excited by the experience and somehow attached their s.e.xuality to it.As these men grew older, the woman's foot became a part of their s.e.xuality. The power and dominance of the woman using her foot was significant to them. They began to fantasize about the thought of being the subject under the woman's foot. They fantasized about the power of the woman, and how she would be able to crush the life out of them if she chose to do so. Many of these men love to be trampled by women. Some like to be trampled by a woman wearing shoes or high heels. Others like to be trampled by women who are barefoot. They prefer to be hurt and the more indifferent the woman is to their pain, the more exciting it is for them.I have learned that the extreme fantasy for these men is to be trampled or crushed to death under the foot of a powerful woman. Because they would only be able to experience this one time, these men have found a way to transfer their fantasy and excitement. They have learned that if they watch a woman crush an animal or live creature to his death, they can fantasize that they are that animal experiencing death at the foot of this woman.27 Congressman Gallegly also heard much the same thing from Jeff Vilencia during their encounter on Court TV. "The viewer identifies with the victim," Jeff stated flatly in an attempt to counter Gallegly's alarming claim that crush freaks were dangerous s.a.d.i.s.ts. The fetish "starts in childhood, when the child observes an adult person, usually a woman, stepping on an insect," he continued, echoing Creede, his legal nemesis. "He becomes s.e.xually aroused, sort of by happenstance, and as he becomes an adolescent, he eroticizes his behavior in his s.e.xuality, and it becomes a part of his love map-" ("His love map love map?" interrupted the host incredulously.) Without an origin story, how else can Jeff refute Gallegly's fantasies of the crush freak as the protoserial killer and the insect as the proto-baby? There's no s.p.a.ce for refusal. At the center of a frighteningly unstable public debate (TV host: "Jeff Vilencia, are you a tiny bit afraid you're going to be prosecuted? You're the one producing these films"), explanation is Jeff's only option. He has to explain that his fetish has a specific history that ties it to specific objects, that it is m.a.s.o.c.h.i.s.tic, and that masochism-as the philospher Gilles Deleuze argues in his famous discussion of Sacher-Masoch-is not a complementary form of sadism but part of an entirely distinct formation. A "s.a.d.i.s.t could never tolerate a m.a.s.o.c.h.i.s.tic victim," writes Deleuze, and "neither would the m.a.s.o.c.h.i.s.t tolerate a truly s.a.d.i.s.tic torturer." The distinctions abound: the m.a.s.o.c.h.i.s.t demands a ritualized fantasy, he creates an anxiety-filled suspense, he exhibits his humiliation, he demands punishment to resolve his anxiety and heighten his forbidden pleasure, and-quite unlike the s.a.d.i.s.t and with Sacher-Masoch as the locus cla.s.sicus-he needs a binding contract with his torturess, who, through the contract (which, in truth, is no more than the m.a.s.o.c.h.i.s.t's word), becomes the embodiment of law.28 But if only it really were that straightforward. When, in a reflective moment, Jeff tells me that the one thing he learned from all of this was that "women are really cruel, just really evil, man," and he says it bleakly, with not a hint of teasing, I see that I haven't quite got the hang of this, and I'm not sure that Deleuze has either. Isn't the point that cruelty is part of a compact, that there is agreement, tacit or explicit, about boundaries and needs? Isn't that what all Jeff's banter with Elizabeth and Mich.e.l.le is probing? Admittedly, at the end of Sacher-Masoch's Venus in Furs Venus in Furs, Wanda lets loose her Greek lover on Severin with the full force of his hunting whips. It's horrible, and unexpected too. But it's also a momentous effort on her part to finally break his dependency, to enforce that break by unambiguously stepping outside the contract. Wanda strains for the gesture that will free them both without the death of either. Under this a.s.sault, writes Sacher-Masoch, Severin "curled up like a worm being crushed." All the poetry is whipped out of him. When it is finally over, he is a changed man. "There is only one alternative," he tells the narrator, "to be the hammer or the anvil." Henceforth, he'll be the one doing the whipping.29 But could this be what happened to Jeff too? Only differently? Did the furies of Fox News strip him of his pleasures? Was that tense but playful self somehow swept away in the storm of disgust that poured down on him? The wrong kind of suffering. Game over, lights on. Cruelty is suddenly just cruelty, Mich.e.l.le is just Mich.e.l.le, stomping on animals and maybe actually enjoying it. No longer a G.o.ddess, no longer exciting. Just mean.
Such a long road to get here. A road paved with explanation. Explanation was straightforward for investigator Susan Creede. Her task before the House subcommittee was to manufacture an object that could be acted upon by the law. Think of her as the forensic expert narrating the corpse. But it's so much more complicated for Jeff Vilencia. It's not simply the demands of the moment that force him to speak Creede's language. Among the DVDs, videos, books, audiotapes, unpublished writings, and press clippings he sent me when we first talked was an unexpected item: a three-page article he'd written called "Fetishes/ Paraphilia/Perversions." The essay begins programmatically: "Perversions are unusual or important modifications of the expected pattern of s.e.xual arousal. One form is fetis.h.i.+sm, of which crush fetis.h.i.+sm is an example." It goes on to describe seven theories of fetish formation ("oxytocin theory," "faulty s.e.xualization theory," "lack of available female contact theory," and so on) and includes as an appendix a discussion of the seventeen "possible stages of fetish development basic to the modified conditioning theory" which both Jeff and Susan Creede used to explain the birth of the crush freak to Congressman Gallegly.
At the time I didn't understand why Jeff wanted me to have this essay. Nor did I understand why he had dedicated Smush Smush to Richard von Krafft-Ebing, the pioneering nineteenth-century Viennese s.e.xologist whose to Richard von Krafft-Ebing, the pioneering nineteenth-century Viennese s.e.xologist whose Psychopathia s.e.xualis Psychopathia s.e.xualis doc.u.mented "aberrant" s.e.xual practices as medical phenomena. But then I got to the second volume of doc.u.mented "aberrant" s.e.xual practices as medical phenomena. But then I got to the second volume of The American Journal of the Crush-Freaks The American Journal of the Crush-Freaks, published in 1996, which is subt.i.tled: "Just where do we fit in, in all of this?" In the introduction, Jeff writes: Imagine all of the shame you would feel having no one to talk to about your desires. It is as alone as one could feel. You might as well be stranded on an island someplace.Well those dark days are behind us now. As we move towards the 21st century, and as various s.e.xual groups have come out over the years, I cannot see any reason that people should feel shame any more....There are more choices today than there ever were. When I was a kid, I was a crush-freak, only I didn't know what to call myself. Today, I know who I am, and more important, I know why why I am. I am.
It was that why why that mattered: "As a child I had a knack for getting in trouble. As an adult I look forward to telling the world my s.e.xuality. I am willing to take all my critics on. I have appeared on television and radio, major newspapers, and adult fetish magazines. I have spoken at four different universities in Southern California. I also produce videotapes that I sell to my fellow crush-freaks for masturbation purposes. Trouble comes to town and his name is Jeff Vilencia!" that mattered: "As a child I had a knack for getting in trouble. As an adult I look forward to telling the world my s.e.xuality. I am willing to take all my critics on. I have appeared on television and radio, major newspapers, and adult fetish magazines. I have spoken at four different universities in Southern California. I also produce videotapes that I sell to my fellow crush-freaks for masturbation purposes. Trouble comes to town and his name is Jeff Vilencia!"30
When Bill Clinton signed H.R. 1887 into law on December 9, 1999, he issued a statement instructing the Justice Department to construe the act narrowly, as applicable only to "wanton cruelty to animals designed to appeal to a prurient interest in s.e.x."31 In the years since, perhaps wary of the legislation's frailty, prosecutors have used it only three times. On each occasion-and contrary to Clinton's directive-they wielded it against distributors of dog-fighting videos. In July 2008, a federal appeals court struck down the law, agreeing with Robert Scott that the First Amendment does not allow the government to ban depictions of illegal conduct (as opposed to that conduct itself). In October 2009, the Supreme Court heard an appeal from the government backed by animal rights groups. In the years since, perhaps wary of the legislation's frailty, prosecutors have used it only three times. On each occasion-and contrary to Clinton's directive-they wielded it against distributors of dog-fighting videos. In July 2008, a federal appeals court struck down the law, agreeing with Robert Scott that the First Amendment does not allow the government to ban depictions of illegal conduct (as opposed to that conduct itself). In October 2009, the Supreme Court heard an appeal from the government backed by animal rights groups.32 No matter the fate of H.R. 1887, for Jeff Vilencia there is no going back from those few hot weeks in the fall of 1999. Jeff tells me how his radio and TV interviews were edited to make him monstrous, how he tried to play the unedited tapes for his family but they believed only the broadcast versions. He tells me how his niece-in the name of her new baby-led his mother on a tour of websites that featured crush videos or specifically targeted him. "I lost friends, my brothers and sisters ... I mean, it was just a horrible ordeal. I was almost isolated. I mean, I had no friends, no one wanted to talk to me, you know, and I just thought ... you know ..." He trailed off. He tells me how he quit video making entirely. "I just got to the point of giving up on life." He tells me how it's since been impossible to find a job because these days employers automatically Google their applicants.
So here we are on the featureless patio outside that suburban Starbucks, and he's telling me that he learned two things from all this: that women are "really cruel b.i.t.c.hes" and that "perversions are best left in the closet, buried in the dark." He's telling me about the men who were inspired by his example to come out to their girlfriends and wives and how in all cases their lives fell to pieces. And he's telling me that he needs love too and that now he has someone who gives it to him but doesn't want him talking about all this anymore. And he checks my watch nervously, leans back in his chair, looks out across the parking lot, sighs, and says softly: "Seems like a dream, man ..."
Go to YouTube, search for "crush videos," and see what you get. There's a lot up there. Short, poor-quality home videos of women stepping on crickets, worms, snails, lots of squishy soft fruit. Some of the clips have had tens of thousands of views, most have a few thousand, one has a couple of hundred thousand. It's a long way from the underground trade of expensive Super 8 film in the 1950s and '60s and even from the back-of-the-p.o.r.n-mags sales in the '80s and '90s. If YouTube doesn't meet your need, it's easy to find longer and more professional product on the many specialist websites that are trading entirely openly despite Gallegly's legislation.
Gallegly himself allowed for this future. At one point in the debate on H.R. 1887, he took the floor to clarify a crucial point. "This has nothing to do with bugs and insects and c.o.c.kroaches, things things like that," he told his colleagues. "This has to do with like that," he told his colleagues. "This has to do with living animals living animals like kittens, monkeys, hamsters, and so on and so forth." like kittens, monkeys, hamsters, and so on and so forth."33 For a moment, there's something on which everyone can agree. There are animals that matter, and there are things that don't. And then Gallegly draws breath, and before you know it, he's off again about Ted Bundy, the Unabomber, and the safety of our children. For a moment, there's something on which everyone can agree. There are animals that matter, and there are things that don't. And then Gallegly draws breath, and before you know it, he's off again about Ted Bundy, the Unabomber, and the safety of our children.
Temptation
It was August 1877, and Baron Carl Robert Osten-Sacken, a Russian aristocrat recently retired as the czar's consul general in New York, had stopped for a few days at Gurnigel, "the well-known watering-place near Bern."1 The baron was forty-nine years old and at a turning point in his life. He would spend a year traveling in Europe before arriving in Cambridge, Ma.s.sachusetts, where, once more in the New World but freed from imperial service, he would settle at Harvard's famous Museum of Comparative Zoology, and spend the rest of his days pursuing his pa.s.sion for flies. Some thirty years later, his obituarist would describe him as "the beau ideal beau ideal of a scientific entomologist," citing his mastery of relevant languages, his independence of means, his elevated social rank, his prodigious memory, exceptional observation skills, "almost perfect" library of works on the Diptera, and naturally, his impeccable manners. of a scientific entomologist," citing his mastery of relevant languages, his independence of means, his elevated social rank, his prodigious memory, exceptional observation skills, "almost perfect" library of works on the Diptera, and naturally, his impeccable manners.2 Strolling one morning in the Alpine woods behind his hotel, the baron's eye was caught by something quite new to him, something he suspected was "unique in entomology." It was not yet ten o'clock, but the sun was already high in the sky. Above his head, zigzagging among the shafts of light slicing through the shadows of the fir trees, were swarms of tiny flies. "What attracted my attention," he wrote that October in an enthusiastic note from Frankfurt, was "the uncommonly brilliant white or silvery reflection which they gave in crossing the sunbeam."
The baron gave chase with his net, caught one with his forceps, and was "astonished to find a much smaller fly than I had expected, and without anything silvery about it." The insect he held was dull gray and thoroughly unremarkable looking.
Tiny things can be slow to reveal their secrets. But Baron Osten-Sacken's exceptional observation skills yielded a clue: "I perceived on the gauze of my forceps, not far from the fly, a flake of opaque, white film-like substance, oval, about 2 mm. long, and so light, that the faintest breath of air could lift it." He thinks of the fine silk spun by ballooning spiders preparing for takeoff. "But for its much lesser weight, it might also be compared to the petal of a small white flower." He catches another, then another, and each time pulls from his net a male fly clutching under its body the same diaphanous structure. He concludes that "these bits of white tissue, which they waved like flags behind them," are the source of the brilliant reflection. But he has no idea what they are or why the flies carry them.
The baron was the first entomologist to encounter balloon flies, as these insects came to be known. But he was by no means the last. In the decades following his discovery, more and more of these animals were described. All turned out to be male. All carried an object of some kind. And all of them belonged to the Empididae, the so-called dance flies, famous for their huge flittering swarms.
In 1955, Edward Kessel, a.s.sociate curator of insects at the California Academy of Sciences, wrote a definitive paper on the balloon empidids in which he suggested that the baron and his successors had been unlucky enough to have stumbled upon a limit case.3 It was as if, knowing only European painting of the late nineteenth century, these gentlemen entomologists had wandered into the art museum and met a wall of Mark Rothkos. They had come upon an abstraction, an inscrutable object that betrayed no trace of the original from which it was distilled. Really, encountered like this, these flakes of white tissue could have been anything, anything at all, even the "aeronautical surfboards" proposed by Josef Mik in 1888. It was as if, knowing only European painting of the late nineteenth century, these gentlemen entomologists had wandered into the art museum and met a wall of Mark Rothkos. They had come upon an abstraction, an inscrutable object that betrayed no trace of the original from which it was distilled. Really, encountered like this, these flakes of white tissue could have been anything, anything at all, even the "aeronautical surfboards" proposed by Josef Mik in 1888.
But over time, Kessel wrote, observers noticed that the male balloon fly always presented his substance to a female fly and, soon after, the two of them had s.e.x. Rather coyly, entomologists called these objects "nuptial gifts," a euphemism still widely used today. Some of the gifts consisted simply of an unadorned dead insect, in others the corpse was bound in frothy or silky tissue (sometimes casually, sometimes carefully), and in some there was no body at all, the elaborate wrapping itself being the offering.
Kessel created an evolutionary history of the empidid gift. He described a hierarchy of species defined by their gift-giving habits, from the primitive to the urbane, the crude to the refined. It was an eight-stage history in which the object in question evolved from something obvious in the most material way (food) to something subtle, recondite, and arguably immaterial (a symbol).4 Empidids are predatory carnivores, and much as it is for praying mantises and many spiders, their s.e.x life is a fraught affair. The way Kessel tells it, the males have become calculating cynics, the females capricious and, as luck would have it, easily distracted. His males-no surprise here-do just about anything for s.e.x; his females, material girls all the way, do whatever it takes to get the bauble. It's very mid-1950s, very Gentlemen Prefer Blondes Gentlemen Prefer Blondes, very film noir too, except that instead of taking place in a nightclub, this is all happening in what biologists call a lek, an arena where the males perform for attention and the females get to pick from the a.s.sembled "eligible bachelors."
The stakes are high. Kessel's males are c.o.c.ky and cunning, but edgy and anxious too. They're calibrating for optimum mileage, making sure their temptations cost as little as possible but still do the job. They're really good dancers. They're constantly watching their backs. Kessel was right that Osten-Sacken could never have figured this out.
Kessel found species of empidids for each of his eight evolutionary stages. From the most primitive, in which "the male does not bear a wedding gift for his bride," to a second stage, in which he carries "a wedding present in the form of a juicy insect," to a third, in which "the prey has become the stimulus for mating," to a fourth, in which "the prey is more or less entangled in silky threads."
Kessel and his wife, Berta, found the fifth stage in Marin County, north of San Francisco, and named it Empis bullifera Empis bullifera for the sticky bubbles it uses to make its container. They spent the summer of 1949 observing mating pairs "drifting in lazy flight, first in one direction and then in another, back and forth within the clearing under the trees, their glistening white balloons producing flashes whenever they penetrate a sunbeam." They watched the animals meet in the air, watched them embrace, and watched the males present the females with balloons containing a midge, a spider, or a tiny psocid. Then they wrote an article together announcing the new species, which they published in 1951 in the for the sticky bubbles it uses to make its container. They spent the summer of 1949 observing mating pairs "drifting in lazy flight, first in one direction and then in another, back and forth within the clearing under the trees, their glistening white balloons producing flashes whenever they penetrate a sunbeam." They watched the animals meet in the air, watched them embrace, and watched the males present the females with balloons containing a midge, a spider, or a tiny psocid. Then they wrote an article together announcing the new species, which they published in 1951 in the Wasmann Journal of Biology. Wasmann Journal of Biology.
In the sixth and seventh stages, the male has sucked the prey dry before giving it to his partner. All she receives is an inedible husk. Nonetheless, the familiar sequence unfolds: the flies embrace, the balloon changes hands (well, legs actually), and s.e.x ensues. The eighth, and final, stage was the one witnessed by the baron. Among Hilara sartor Hilara sartor and a few other species, the enigmatic gift contains no prey at all, not even the withered sh.e.l.l. and a few other species, the enigmatic gift contains no prey at all, not even the withered sh.e.l.l.
Kessel emphasized distinctions among species. So do contemporary biologists, but they also recognize a good deal of within-species variation. They describe dance fly species in which males offer both large and small presents and others in which they offer both edible and inedible ones. They also describe males whose prey gifts are hunted from their own species and others who eschew insect prey altogether to gather entirely different gifts-flower petals, for example. Despite this variety of behavior, the small number of researchers who study these little flies still closely follow Kessel's account of an evolutionary Diptera economicus Diptera economicus, in which males do everything they can to minimize their energy output and maximize their reproductive return, relentlessly downgrading their gifts in the effort to get cheaper and cheaper s.e.x.
This subst.i.tution of "empty gifts" for nutritious ones has become a famous case of "male cheating," an idea that doesn't require just the males' shrewd duplicity but also relies on the females' slowness.5 Even when the gifts are "worthless," even when they're just the cheapest gimcracks-plain cotton b.a.l.l.s provided by biologists, for example-the researchers describe the foolish female fly giving the fake-gift giver what he wants, or at least the researchers see the female taking so long to catch on to the phoniness of her reward that she's given her partner what he wants before she realizes she's had nothing in return. Tricked. Deceived. f.u.c.ked. Over and over and over. Even when the gifts are "worthless," even when they're just the cheapest gimcracks-plain cotton b.a.l.l.s provided by biologists, for example-the researchers describe the foolish female fly giving the fake-gift giver what he wants, or at least the researchers see the female taking so long to catch on to the phoniness of her reward that she's given her partner what he wants before she realizes she's had nothing in return. Tricked. Deceived. f.u.c.ked. Over and over and over.
Or so the story goes.
Writing about Ellis Island, the French novelist Georges Perec found himself haunted by what might have been, by what he called "potential memory."
"It concerns me, it fascinates me," he wrote, "it involves me, it questions me." Broken by the deportation of his mother from Paris to Auschwitz when he was six years old, Perec kept running into histories that could have been his own, a boy who veers down a monochrome side street half a block ahead, "a life-story that might have been mine," "a probable autobiobiography," "a memory that might have belonged to me," books full of absences, a novel without the letter e e, a novella that lacks the other vowels: a, i, o a, i, o, and u. u.6 These fictive histories aren't just imaginative games. They're hard facts of existence that weigh down the present with paths not taken. We all have them: the discarded negatives of decisions whose weight only later becomes clear, the psychic resonances that complete the life we do have, the life that does get made. When Sharon s.h.i.+vers without warning, she says, "Someone just walked over my grave."
Kessel saw that Osten-Sacken's flies flittered not only in the open forest but also in a narrative vacuum. How could the baron and those who followed him take these inscrutable flakes-like petals but so much less substantial-and build a story when there was no history? What could they do when none of the signs signified, when all they had was gossamer? The beauty touched them. But that just made it worse. Without even potential histories, how could they understand what might be, what might have been, and what now was?
But the opposite problem is equally real. The problem of too much history. How can one understand what might be, what might have been, and what now is when confronted by a history so potent that it makes different or simply more expansive stories so hard to imagine?
Of course, it is possible that male empidids are cheats and that female empidids are dopes. But it's also possible that male and female dance flies are not constantly at war with each other and are not always caught up in relations.h.i.+ps drawn straight out of daytime soaps.
"Perhaps animals do lie to each other now and then," writes the evolutionary biologist Joan Roughgarden, "but biologists have yet to catch them in a lie."7 She presumes that animals are honest until proven devious and that they have capacity until proven incapable. What if, like her, we a.s.sume that those female flies know what they are doing? What if those empty balloons actually are gifts-but gifts whose value we don't understand? Perhaps these tiny structures possess an ecstatic tactility. Perhaps they're seductively comforting. Perhaps they trigger memories or appet.i.tes. Perhaps they're symbolically precious, full of affect and meaning. Perhaps the flies just like them. She presumes that animals are honest until proven devious and that they have capacity until proven incapable. What if, like her, we a.s.sume that those female flies know what they are doing? What if those empty balloons actually are gifts-but gifts whose value we don't understand? Perhaps these tiny structures possess an ecstatic tactility. Perhaps they're seductively comforting. Perhaps they trigger memories or appet.i.tes. Perhaps they're symbolically precious, full of affect and meaning. Perhaps the flies just like them.
How do we avoid turning the empidids into one of Stephen Jay Gould's evolutionary just-so stories, stories that can't be validated or falsified because they begin from the conviction of a mechanism-in this case, s.e.xual selection driven by s.e.xual conflict-and shoehorn whatever data emerge from experiment into a preexisting argument? What, for instance, if we leave open the possibility that there is a diversity of relations.h.i.+ps among these flies that corresponds to the diversity of behaviors that biologists have already observed? What if we a.s.sume that the willingness of many female flies to accept cotton b.a.l.l.s indicates that rather than being "worthless," the objects have qualities unknown to us? Is it too obvious to point out (again) the hazards of presuming what an object is and what it does for beings whose ways of being are so different from our own?
It is August 1877, and Carl Robert Osten-Sacken stands among the trees behind his hotel in Gurnigel, shading his eyes as he gazes up into the dappled sunlight and marvels at the dazzling flashes of Hilara sartor Hilara sartor dancing in and out of the sunbeams. dancing in and out of the sunbeams.
It is summer 1949, and Edward and Berta Kessel keep impossibly still in Marin County, California, so as not to disturb the mating Empis bullifera Empis bullifera and especially not the female exploring her delicately wrapped present. and especially not the female exploring her delicately wrapped present.
It is May 2004, and on a farm in Fife, Scotland, Natasha LeBas uses her forceps to prize a dead insect from the clutch of a female Rhamphomyia sulcata Rhamphomyia sulcata and, hoping against hope not to interrupt the flies' coitus, carefully swaps the prey for a cotton ball. and, hoping against hope not to interrupt the flies' coitus, carefully swaps the prey for a cotton ball.
It is mid-2010 or further into the future, and here we are again, caught between the unavoidability of comparison and the awareness of fundamental difference. Here we are still, caught in that imperative to understand, bearing our various tools for a.n.a.lysis and interpretation, trying to locate both the objective principles and the lived life in the enigmatic clues of observed behavior. Here we are again, caught somewhere between the reduction that makes things fathomable and the generosity that gives them fullness. Here we are, caught looking yet again, still looking, this time at tiny flies and their s.h.i.+ny gifts.
The U Unseen
Sometimes late at night I hear rustling. I work here, upstairs in my room, writing this book at the top of the building, perched on the roof, thinking about insects and all the things they make and do, sitting at my desk in this squat box coated black with pitch to protect against the city rain.
There are grilles on the windows. But there's also a sliding door, and if you step outside onto the squishy silver-coated tar and look to the left, the sweep of the Hudson will catch your breath, especially in winter, when the trees are bare and the lights of New Jersey glitter in the river's laquered blackness.
In the daytime, white egrets and red-tailed hawks fly past on their way to Central Park. Cardinals, finches, blue jays, squeaky mourning doves, and raggedy pigeons perch on our railing. At dusk, the sparrows go crazy in the trees below. A little later, Sharon and I head downstairs into Riverside Park, down past the Amtrak tunnel where Brooklyn (six years in the Marines, twenty-four on the streets) sleeps with her cats and racc.o.o.ns, and where we watch the urban wildlife forage garbage in the gloom of the streetlamps.
It's peaceful upstairs in this blanket of quiet. Night falls, and one by one the lights go out in the surrounding apartment buildings. The rush of traffic on the Westside Highway subsides. The last planes pa.s.s overhead. The quiet deepens, and we all head into the darkness.
Up here, at the top of the building, I turn my desk light down. I lower the brightness on my laptop. My eyes struggle against the dimness, then relax. Everything slows.
Sometimes in summer, when it's hot and humid, the night is interrupted by rustling. It's not the mice in the drywall or the squirrels in the guttering. It's not the hairy centipedes that scamper into the corners. It's not the mosquitoes or bluebottles or those erratic crane flies. It's not the ladybugs or winged ants that arrive each year en ma.s.se without warning and just as abruptly disappear. It's not the building stretching in the breeze. It's not the leaves blowing up against the windows. It's not a mystery. I know what it is. It's the big water bugs, the American c.o.c.kroaches, come to scratch along the walls, doing what they do, going from place to place, up from the drains, not really wanting to be here, a bit lost, looking for something.
Kikuo Itaya, the twentieth-century Zen Buddhist short story writer, lived among c.o.c.kroaches, refusing to harm them, allowing them to share his home. But he was unusual, even in j.a.pan. I think of him when I kill them. I have to kill them because Sharon is phobic; she freaks out when she sees one: she hides, she shakes, her body goes into spasm. Once she's seen one, I can't just pretend to have killed it. If I do, it will only break cover again, and everything will be worse than before. And anyway, she knows when I lie.
When I hear the scratching, I turn the lights down even further. My skin crawls in antic.i.p.ation. If she doesn't see it, if I don't see it, if it remains unseen ... I don't want to know it's there.
But sometimes the scratches are too insistent. One night, distracted and without thinking, I swiveled around. A healthy-looking water bug was sitting on a pile of books behind my shoulder. We locked eyes. Its head extended like a turtle's. Its face was angular and inquisitive. Really, as Karl von Frisch once remarked, it had "the lofty brow of the philosopher."1 Our eyes met as in an animal movie. An understanding beyond words. But I must have moved too suddenly, and it took off and I took off after it, grabbing a broom-everything all of a sudden kinetic-trapping it in a cluttered corner, its legs a whir of mad scrambling, and caught up in the moment, I beat it and beat it, until I realized I was trembling and disgusted and confused and it was just a smush of fat and chitin on the wooden floor. "Just a grease spot," as Erika Elizondo would say. Our eyes met as in an animal movie. An understanding beyond words. But I must have moved too suddenly, and it took off and I took off after it, grabbing a broom-everything all of a sudden kinetic-trapping it in a cluttered corner, its legs a whir of mad scrambling, and caught up in the moment, I beat it and beat it, until I realized I was trembling and disgusted and confused and it was just a smush of fat and chitin on the wooden floor. "Just a grease spot," as Erika Elizondo would say.
I keep the lights low and the shadows deep. I know it's there, but I can't see it. If I don't see it, we're safe. The night protects us both. When the rustling begins, I don't turn around. If all goes well, eventually it stops, and not long after, the birds start singing, just a few at first, then more, and louder, until, as the dawn rises and the sun fills the room, they come louder still.
But then, this morning actually, something new happened. I was in the shower, daydreaming as usual under the soothing warm water, thoughts rambling around the chapter of this book I'm trying to finish-the one about queer insects and the queer things they like to do-when, out of nowhere, a three-inch water bug dropped from the bathroom ceiling and landed at my feet.
I admit it: I screamed. Wouldn't you? I shut off the water. It took a moment to get over the surprise. And then there we were, the water bug and I, trapped and defenseless and covered in soapsuds. And we both stayed very still until that very big little animal, a female animal, I noticed, climbed swiftly up onto the towel rack and stopped there at eye level a few inches away, her handsome and intelligent face c.o.c.ked at a philosophical angle, giving me a funny, quizzical look up and down as if amused by this unexpected situation and intrigued to see what would happen next. One of us was very calm. One of us-it was the bathroom, after all-began carefully to groom her antennae. I won't go into the details of what happened next. I'm not sure even Erika Elizondo would have felt good this time.
Vision
Academy Studios, an exhibition design and fabrication company based in Novato, California, created these interactive stations for the Arthropod Zoo at the North Carolina State Museum of Natural Sciences. They built a seven-foot praying mantis and a dragonfly with a twelve-foot wingspan-both anatomically accurate!-but it's these masks that get the most attention, spooky-looking sci-fi helmets that, as Academy's promotional materials put it, "give visitors a chance to see life through a bee's eyes."
Robert Yagura, then Academy's creative director, told me that they used hexagonal pieces of Lucite to mimic the facets of a bee's compound eye and bonded them to a curved form to produce a fractured image. But even with that prosthetic, Robert told me, the visitor does not see like a bee. For a start, a bee's sensitivity to the electromagnetic spectrum is s.h.i.+fted significantly to wavelengths shorter than those visible to a human. At the low end, below 380 nanometers, bees are able to make out ultraviolet light invisible to us; at the high end, they're red-blind, red appearing to them as an empty blackness, the absence of light.
The little-remembered zoologist Charles Henry Turner shares credit with Karl von Frisch for providing the first glimpse of the world through the eyes of a bee.1 Turner, the first African American to receive a Ph.D. from the University of Chicago and the author of more than fifty research papers, published his account in 1910, at the beginning of a long career as a science teacher in public high schools. Von Frisch completed his studies in 1913, well before he became director of the Inst.i.tute of Zoology at Munich and witnessed the honeybees' dances. He was already driven by the impulse to demonstrate the capacities of his tiny friends that would eventually win him the n.o.bel Prize. Despite the extravagance of floral color and the intricate economies of codependence that tie insects and angiosperms across the millennia, before Turner and von Frisch had given the matter their attention, it was commonly thought that insects were entirely color-blind. Turner, the first African American to receive a Ph.D. from the University of Chicago and the author of more than fifty research papers, published his account in 1910, at the beginning of a long career as a science teacher in public high schools. Von Frisch completed his studies in 1913, well before he became director of the Inst.i.tute of Zoology at Munich and witnessed the honeybees' dances. He was already driven by the impulse to demonstrate the capacities of his tiny friends that would eventually win him the n.o.bel Prize. Despite the extravagance of floral color and the intricate economies of codependence that tie insects and angiosperms across the millennia, before Turner and von Frisch had given the matter their attention, it was commonly thought that insects were entirely color-blind.
Von Frisch's refutation is famous-and characteristically and elegantly low-tech. He set out a series of dishes on cards. In just one square, the only blue card in a field of varying grays, the dish contained sugar water. He began by training his bees to visit this card and dish. Then, over several hours, he varied their position in the matrix. Next, he removed all the cards and dishes, replacing them with a new set of identical materials, only now leaving the dish on the blue card empty. As he expected, the bees returned to the blue card, attracted by color rather than odor or location.2 As von Frisch explained, this behavior demonstrated the bees' "true color sense," not simply their ability to distinguish among light intensities. If their vision were monochromatic, he pointed out, they would have found at least some of the gray cards indistinguishable from the blue. As von Frisch explained, this behavior demonstrated the bees' "true color sense," not simply their ability to distinguish among light intensities. If their vision were monochromatic, he pointed out, they would have found at least some of the gray cards indistinguishable from the blue.3 There's little argument nowadays that most insects see in some form of color. Through electrophysiologic experiments on photoreceptive cells, researchers can readily demonstrate the capacity for color vision. They know, for example, that bees, like humans, are trichromatic, possessing three types of photosensitive pigments that absorb maximally in different parts of the spectrum (though in green, blue, and ultraviolet rather than our red, green, and blue). And they also know-though there are few ways of conceiving what it might actually mean-that dragonflies and b.u.t.terflies are often pentachromatic, possessing five types of pigments. (They also know that mantid shrimp have receptors sensitive at twelve twelve different wavelengths!) different wavelengths!) It is one thing, however, to demonstrate that animals possess the capacity for color vision, quite another to show that the world through which they move glistens and glimmers, as does ours, in multiple hues. For this, researchers rely on behavioral studies, and they still use the techniques pioneered by Turner and von Frisch, training their animals to respond to food rewards and colored patches.
But insects can be recalcitrant research subjects, and so far this type of work has been carried out only among honeybees, blowflies, and a few species of b.u.t.terflies.4 Given the distinctive absorption spectra of these animals' photoreceptors, we can be fairly certain that objects will appear quite different to them than they do to us. Many flowers, for example, look quite different when viewed through an ultraviolet filter. These black-eyed susans ( Given the distinctive absorption spectra of these animals' photoreceptors, we can be fairly certain that objects will appear quite different to them than they do to us. Many flowers, for example, look quite different when viewed through an ultraviolet filter. These black-eyed susans (Rudbeckia hirta) display a bull's-eye pattern that seems to guide bees, wasps, and other pollinators to their target; other flowers have a characteristic runway pattern leading to the same destination.
This is so simple yet so intriguing. There are invisible worlds all around us, parallel worlds. Familiar objects have secret ident.i.ties, some of which we can access through straightforward mechanical tricks, like Lucite fractals and UV filters, but others of which remain inaccessible, even to our imagination (twelve pigments?). We pa.s.s through not just purblind but enc.u.mbered by the everyday a.s.sumption that the world we see is the world that is. In this respect at least, our perception is rather shallow, though I admit it's unlikely bees or b.u.t.terflies are any more decentered. pigments?). We pa.s.s through not just purblind but enc.u.mbered by the everyday a.s.sumption that the world we see is the world that is. In this respect at least, our perception is rather shallow, though I admit it's unlikely bees or b.u.t.terflies are any more decentered.
Nonetheless, at the very least, the natural world's indifference should make us wary of a.s.suming too quickly that flowers that draw our eye are similarly seductive to a pollinator. Such hidden truths make visible one important fact about vision (our own and that of other beings): it is a property not only of the viewer and the object but also of the relations.h.i.+p between them.5
The closer we look, the more we see. Bee masks and UV photos are not just intriguing; they're beguiling. If we could only re-create an insect's visual apparatus, they promise, we could see what it sees, and if we could see what what it sees ... why, then we could see it sees ... why, then we could see as as it sees, too. But I doubt many of us, including scientists and exhibit designers, believe this. Vision is so much more than mechanics. it sees, too. But I doubt many of us, including scientists and exhibit designers, believe this. Vision is so much more than mechanics.
The Soviet entomologist Georgii Mazokhin-Porshnyakov drew attention to this long ago: "When we talk about vision," he wrote in the late 1950s, "we imply not only that animals are able to distinguish objects visually (i.e., the stimuli), but also that they are able to recognize them."6 Photoreception, on its own, he suggested, is of little value to an organism; what counts is the ability to identify an object and make some sense of it. Reception presupposes perception. Insects see with their brain, not their eyes. Photoreception, on its own, he suggested, is of little value to an organism; what counts is the ability to identify an object and make some sense of it. Reception presupposes perception. Insects see with their brain, not their eyes.
In this respect, an insect's vision is identical to that of a human. Like ours, an insect's vision is a complex sorting procedure, a way to filter and hierarchize objects in the world, one sense among several interdependent senses, one entangled element of perception.
Frederick Prete, a biologist at DePaul University who studies the visual universe of praying mantises, points out that until quite recently the standard scientific a.s.sumption was that insect vision operated by exclusion, that bees, b.u.t.terflies, wasps, mantises, and similar creatures were designed "to ignore all but some very limited, specific types of visual information ... [such as] a small, moving, fly-shaped spot just a few millimeters away ... [or] yellow flowers of a certain size." Instead, Prete and his colleagues demonstrate, mantises and many other insects deal with sensory information in ways not dissimilar from those of humans: "they use categories to cla.s.sify moving objects; [and] they learn and use complex algorithms to solve difficult problems." Prete describes human processing of visual information as a type of taxonomy: We filter sensory information by recognizing and a.s.sessing certain key characteristics of the events and objects around us, and we use that information to identify an event or object as an example of a general cla.s.s of events or objects. For instance, you would not reject a meal ... because it did not look like a specific, idealized plate of food. You would a.s.sess its characteristics (odor, color, texture, temperature), and if they all met certain criteria, you would take a bite. In this case, the novel meal is an example of the category "acceptable meal." Likewise, we can learn that a particular task-mending a ripped curtain, for instance-is an example of the category "sewing material together." So, when attempting to mend a curtain for the first time, we apply the rules that we learned are successful in other, a.n.a.logous mending tasks. In other words, we have acquired and employ an algorithm, or "rule of thumb" for solving specific problems of this general type.7 A mantis, write Prete and his colleague Karl Kral, is confronted by a large number of potential meals in the course of a day, and like us, it both creates and deploys a relational category ("a theoretical, perceptual envelope") that corresponds to the thought "acceptable meal." To evaluate an object, the animal draws on experience-learned from past events and encounters-to a.s.sess a series of "stimulus parameters" that include the object's size (if it is compact), its length (if it is elongated), the contrast between the object and its background, the object's location in the mantis's visual field, the object's speed, and the object's overall direction of movement.8 A varying number of these criteria must be met for the mantis to strike. Yet, rather than a reaction being triggered by the meeting of a specific threshold, the mantis takes into account the relations.h.i.+p among different data in each parameter. Kral and Prete call this calculation a "perceptual algorithm" (and make the not-unreasonable point that if it were described in primates, it would be considered abstract reasoning). A varying number of these criteria must be met for the mantis to strike. Yet, rather than a reaction being triggered by the meeting of a specific threshold, the mantis takes into account the relations.h.i.+p among different data in each parameter. Kral and Prete call this calculation a "perceptual algorithm" (and make the not-unreasonable point that if it were described in primates, it would be considered abstract reasoning).
Along with a small number of other invertebrate scientists who integrate behavioral and neuroanatomical studies in what is sometimes called psychophysiological research (that is, research on the connections between the psychological and physiological aspects of behavior), Kral and Prete write un-self-consciously of the complexity of insect behavior, of the correspondence between the ways insects and vertebrates (including humans) make sense of the world, and of the insect's mind. mind.
But maybe these insects are just a little too calculating, modeled a little too much on the rational actors of cla.s.sical economic theory (who we know from our own experience don't really exist). Maybe they're not lively and spontaneous enough. How do we know they always calculate solely according to the logic of the hunter? Might they have other desires? Or maybe this is exactly how mantises are even if we don't have to a.s.sume that b.u.t.terflies, say, or fruit flies proceed in this way too. No matter; this is thought-provoking work: there is a cognition here, say Kral and Prete, that is dependent on but somehow not reducible to physiology. Yet, if cognitive processes are irreducible to electrochemical function, what exactly are they? n.o.body seems very sure.9 It's worth noting that these questions are central to contemporary neuroscience, the interdisciplinary field concerned with the study of the nervous systems of animals. Neuroscience is dedicated to physiological explanation but is nonetheless deeply preoccupied with questions of mind, with such indeterminate phenomena as consciousness, cognition, and perception, with material solutions to what many might consider ontological or even metaphysical problems. In neuroscience, it is axiomatic that the brain is the center of all animal life-"the key philosophical theme of modern neural